Research Paper Volume 11, Issue 12 pp 4090—4106
Cognitive compensatory mechanisms in normal aging: a study on verbal fluency and the contribution of other cognitive functions
- 1 Department of Clinical Psychology, Psychobiology and Methodology, Faculty of Psychology, University of La Laguna, La Laguna, Tenerife, Spain
- 2 Division of Clinical Geriatrics, Center for Alzheimer Research, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences, and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
- 3 Department of Neuroimaging, Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
Received: February 16, 2019 Accepted: June 17, 2019 Published: June 22, 2019
https://doi.org/10.18632/aging.102040How to Cite
Copyright: Gonzalez-Burgos et al. This is an open‐access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Abstract
Verbal fluency has been widely studied in cognitive aging. However, compensatory mechanisms that maintain its optimal performance with increasing age are not completely understood. Using cross-sectional data, we investigated differentiation and dedifferentiation processes in verbal fluency across the lifespan by analyzing the association between verbal fluency and numerous cognitive measures within four age groups (N=446): early middle-age (32-45 years), late middle-age (46-58 years), early elderly (59-71 years), and late elderly (72-84 years). ANCOVA was used to investigate the interaction between age and fluency modality. Random forest models were conducted to study the contribution of cognition to semantic, phonemic, and action fluency. All modalities declined with increasing age, but semantic fluency was the most vulnerable to aging. The most prominent reduction in performance was observed during the transition from middle-age to early elderly, when cognitive variables stopped contributing (differentiation), and new cognitive variables started contributing (dedifferentiation). Lexical access, processing speed, and executive functions were among the most contributing functions. We conclude that the association between age and verbal fluency is masked by age-specific influences of other cognitive functions. Differentiation and dedifferentiation processes can coexist. This study provides important data for better understanding of cognitive aging and compensatory processes.
Introduction
Cognitive decline is inherent to the normal aging process. Abilities such as executive functions, processing speed, memory, attention, and visuoconstructive and visuospatial functions decline with age [1–5]. Other functions such as crystallized abilities remain stable or even improve with age [1,6,7].
The effect of aging on language abilities has always attracted a great interest. Language is one of the most complex functions in humans, it is essential for the communication between people, and its impairment has traditionally been a subject of intense study [8]. Interestingly, perhaps due to its strong biological role, studies on normal aging have shown that some language abilities are quite resilient to the onslaught of aging. Comprehension, semantic abilities, and vocabulary remain rather stable or even improve with age [9,10]. Contrarily, other abilities such as verbal fluency and naming are among the most vulnerable cognitive functions to aging [11].
Cognitive tests of verbal fluency measure the ability to produce as many words as possible according to specific rules and a time limit. Phonemic fluency refers to the production of words beginning with a given letter (e.g. “F”). Semantic fluency refers to the production of words belonging to a semantic category (e.g. “animals”). Action fluency refers to the production of words belonging to the grammatical category of verbs (e.g. “to reflect”) [12]. Despite extensive research on the effect of age on verbal fluency, findings are not completely consistent. Numerous studies have shown that semantic fluency declines with age and phonemic fluency seems to be more stable [13–18]. However, contrary results are also common [19,20]. Research on action fluency in normal aging is scarce. Some studies showed lower word production with increasing age [20–22], while other studies showed similar levels of word production with increasing age [12,22,23]. The reason for these contradictory findings is partly related to methodological differences across studies such as the use of different study designs (longitudinal vs. cross-sectional), the sample (size, selection criteria, age groups, age span, etc.), and the statistical approach (correlation vs. means comparison vs. covariance analysis, etc.), among others. In addition, variation on the age span studied has implications beyond mere methodological differences because different compensatory mechanisms may be active and influence fluency performance differently at different ages.
However, compensatory mechanisms have not been investigated in detail. Understanding how diverse cognitive functions contribute to maintain an optimal performance in verbal fluency is of relevance. Previous studies have reported an association of semantic fluency with processing speed [17,24,25], lexical access [25–27], executive functions [26,28], and working memory [25]. Phonemic fluency has been reported to be associated with processing speed [17,24,25], attention [13,29], lexical access [27], executive functions [26,28–31], and memory [29,32]. Studies on action fluency did not find an association with episodic memory or picture naming [12,23]. Whether these associations contribute to compensatory effects across age is unknown. In addition, these associations may change with age. According to the “age differentiation hypothesis” [33], the organizational structure of cognitive abilities changes with age [34]. In particular, cognitive abilities shift from a differentiated condition at younger ages (abilities are separate systems: differentiation), into a dedifferentiated condition at older ages (abilities are more interrelated with each other: dedifferentiation) [35]. This higher intercorrelation proposed by the “age dedifferentiation hypothesis” is associated with reduced neural specificity to cognitive processes as a consequence of biological brain aging and increased interhemispheric activations [35–37]. However, studies addressing the dedifferentiation hypothesis of cognitive aging have generated inconsistent findings, probably due to differences in cognitive abilities assessed, age ranges of the included samples, and analytical techniques used across studies [38]. Further, very few studies have investigated differentiation and dedifferentiation processes on verbal fluency across the whole lifespan [1,17,29,31]. Advancing in our understanding of compensatory mechanisms, differentiation, and dedifferentiation processes is expected to have important implications. In clinical practice, this knowledge could contribute to reach a more accurate diagnosis of cognitive disorders, and could facilitate early and personalized therapeutic interventions. Scientifically, this knowledge may help to better understand age-related processes of the human brain, and its dynamic responses to both negative and positive influences.
The overall purpose of this cross-sectional study was to investigate how differentiation and dedifferentiation processes in verbal fluency are organized across the lifespan. Therefore, we investigated the association between performance in three components of verbal fluency (semantic, phonemic, and action) and performance in numerous non-fluency cognitive measures within different age groups from the early middle-age to the late elderly.
Results
In order to study the association between age and verbal fluency, the sample was divided into four equidistant age groups based on the own sample age distribution: early middle-age (32 to 45.9 years), late middle-age (46 to 58.9 years), early elderly (59 to 71.9 years), and late elderly (72 to 84.9 years). Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics of these age groups.
Table 1. Demographic characteristics and verbal fluency performance.
Early middle-age (n=79) | Late middle-age (n=143) | Early elderly (n=162) | Late elderly (n=62) | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
M(SD)/count(%) | M(SD)/count | M(SD)/count | M(SD)/count | p-value | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Age, years (min-max) | 41.4 (2.8) a,b,c (32-45.9) | 51.0 (3.9) b,c (46-58.9) | 65.6 (3.4) c (59-71.9) | 74.9 (2.3) (72-84.9) | <0.001 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Sex (female, count (%)) | 43 (54.4) | 79 (55.2) | 91 (56.2) | 32 (51.6) | 0.943 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Education level | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Illiteracy Unfinished primary studies Completed primary studies Completed secondary studies University studies | 0 0 34 25 20 | 0 3 52 39 49 | 6 32 52 24 48 | 1 18 24 13 5 | <0.001 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
WAIS-III Information | 15.1 (5.9) a,c | 17.3 (5.8) b,c | 14.5 (6.3) c | 12.4 (5.9) | <0.001 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Semantic fluency | 22.6 (5.6) a,b,c | 22.2 (5.5) b,c | 18.2 (5.2) c | 15.7 (3.8) | <0.001 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Phonemic fluency | 34.8 (10.1) b,c | 37.2 (11.8) b,c | 28.1 (13.8) | 25.8 (10.4) | <0.001 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Action fluency | 18.1 (6.8) b,c | 19.3 (7.6) b,c | 12.9 (7.4) | 10.1 (4.9) | <0.001 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
WAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third edition. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a Significantly different from Late middle-age. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
b Significantly different from Early elderly. c Significantly different from Late elderly. |
Age-related differences on verbal fluency
A mixed ANCOVA model was conducted to examine the interaction between age and verbal fluency. The age groups (4 levels: early middle-age, late middle-age, early elderly, and late elderly) served as the between-subject factor, and the verbal fluency task (3 levels: animals, phonemic, and action fluency) served as the within-subject factor. WAIS-III Information was included as a covariate. This model showed a significant interaction (F(6, 882)=7.8; p<0.001) (Figure 1). There is a clear association between age and verbal fluency, but this association is modulated by the fluency modality. The two middle-age groups do not differ with each other in performance on phonemic and action fluency (p>0.05), and perform rather similar on semantic fluency (p=0.048). However, the two middle-age groups always outperform the two elderly groups (p<0.01). In addition, the early-elderly group outperformed the late-elderly group, but only on semantic fluency (p<0.05) (Table 1). Due to this finding, the two middle-age groups were combined together and only three age groups were used for subsequent analyses (i.e. middle-age, early elderly, and late elderly) (Table 2).
Figure 1. The mixed ANCOVA model for age-related differences on verbal fluency. The x-axis represents the age groups. The y-axis represents the number of words produced. The total number of words produced on phonemic fluency (F+A+S) was divided by three in order to allow comparability among the three fluency modalities (1 minute). P-values are reported for the estimation of linear, quadratic, and cubic effects from the trend analysis tested through the ANCOVA model. The lines represent the outcome from the mixed ANCOVA for age (between-subjects factor) and fluency modality (within-subjects factor) using cross-sectional data.
Table 2. Demographic characteristics and verbal fluency performance in the three age groups.
Middle-age (n=222) | Early elderly (n=162) | Late elderly (n=62) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
M(SD) | M(SD) | M(SD) | p-value | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Age, years (min-max) | 47.6 (5.8) a,b (32-58) | 65.6 (3.4) b (59-71) | 74.9 (2.3) (72-84) | <0.001 | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Sex (female, n (%)) | 122 (55,0) | 91 (56.2) | 32 (51.6) | 0.829 | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
WAIS-III Information | 16.5 (5.9) a,b | 14.5 (6.3) b | 12.4 (5.9) | <0.001 | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Semantic fluency | 22.3 (5.5) a,b | 18.2 (5.2) | 15.7 (3.8) | <0.001 | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Phonemic fluency | 36.4 (11.2) a,b | 28.1 (13.8) | 25.8 (10.4) | <0.001 | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Action fluency | 18.9 (7.4) a,b | 12.9 (7.3) | 10.1 (4.9) | <0.001 | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
WAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third edition. Middle-age = Early middle-age + Late middle-age. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
a Significantly different from Early elderly. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
b Significantly different from Late elderly. |
Contribution of cognitive variables to verbal fluency by age groups
To assess whether the contribution of numerous cognitive variables to verbal fluency differs across age, a random forest regression model was performed separately for each of the three age groups (i.e. middle-age, early elderly, and late elderly). For a description of the cognitive variables (predictors) included in the random forests and their abbreviation please see “Neuropsychological assessment” in the Methods section as well as the Supplementary Table S1.
Table 3 shows that while similar cognitive abilities contributed to verbal fluency across age, some interesting differences can be observed.
Table 3. Contribution of cognitive variables to verbal fluency by age groups (random forest regression models).
Semantic fluency | Phonemic fluency | Action fluency | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ME | EE | LE | Pattern | ME | EE | LE | Pattern | ME | EE | LE | Pattern | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Sample size, n | 222 | 162 | 62 | 222 | 162 | 62 | 222 | 162 | 62 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Explained variance | 27% | 33% | 20% | 35% | 56% | 39% | 43% | 47% | 23% | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Predictors | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
BNT | 22 | 20 | 8 | S/Dif. | 35 | 48 | 17 | S. | 26 | 24 | 8 | S/Dif. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
PCV - Decision time | 7 | 4 | S. | 5 | Dif. | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
PCV - Motor time | 19 | Dif. | 3 | 8 | 2 | S. | S. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
PASAT | 2 | 6 | 2 | S. | 5 | 1 | S. | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
STROOP Words | 9 | 16 | 7 | S. | 30 | 28 | 19 | S. | 29 | 15 | 24 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
STROOP Colors | 11 | 14 | 18 | S. | 19 | 33 | 19 | S. | 21 | 9 | 10 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
STROOP Inhibition | 3 | 15 | 4 | S. | 2 | 20 | 3 | S. | 9 | 6 | 3 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TMT A | 22 | 21 | 16 | S. | 19 | 22 | 14 | S. | 25 | 20 | 8 | S/Dif. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
CTT - Part 1 | 5 | 25 | 19 | S/Ded. | 5 | 32 | 24 | S/Ded. | 17 | 37 | 24 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
CTT - Part 2 | 25 | 9 | 17 | S. | 31 | 13 | Ded. | 10 | 35 | 14 | S. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
FRT | 2 | 3 | Dif. | 3 | Ded. | 4 | Dif. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
JLOT - First half | 5 | 6 | Dif. | 2 | 8 | 5 | S. | 9 | 6 | Ded. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
JLOT - Second half | 5 | 6 | Dif. | 2 | 16 | S/Ded. | 8 | 8 | Dif. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Digit Span forward | 5 | 12 | Dif. | 8 | 20 | 6 | S. | 24 | 10 | 2 | S/Dif. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Digit Span backward | 4 | 13 | Dif. | 35 | 14 | 3 | S/Dif. | 27 | 2 | Dif. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Spatial Span forward | 1 | S/Dif. | 6 | Ded. | 8 | 5 | Dif. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Spatial Span backward | 6 | S. | 5 | 9 | S. | 2 | 10 | 12 | S. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
LM A - Immediate | 2 | 20 | 3 | S. | 5 | 9 | 4 | S. | 8 | 9 | 10 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
LM B1 - Immediate | 13 | 13 | Dif. | 12 | 19 | Dif. | 25 | 11 | 17 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
LM B2 - Immediate | 8 | 10 | Dif. | 7 | 14 | 15 | S. | 19 | 19 | 15 | S. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
LM A - Delay | 3 | 14 | 4 | S. | 5 | 9 | Dif. | 14 | 10 | 3 | S/Dif. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
LM B - Delay | 11 | 10 | Dif. | 7 | 16 | 3 | S. | 18 | 18 | 19 | S. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
LM A - Recognition | 3 | Dif. | 3 | 2 | 6 | S. | 3 | 3 | Dif. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
LM B - Recognition | 7 | 9 | Dif. | 16 | 13 | Dif. | 5 | 10 | 7 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC 1st trial | 5 | 7 | S. | 4 | S. | 1 | S/Ded. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC Learning | 15 | 12 | 22 | S. | 7 | S. | 6 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC Short delay | 6 | 11 | 4 | S. | 8 | S. | 1 | 3 | Ded. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC Short delay-Clues | 10 | 3 | Ded. | S. | S. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC Long delay | 1 | 6 | Ded. | 4 | S. | 5 | S. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC Long delay-Clues | 4 | 8 | 2 | S. | 3 | S. | 2 | 9 | Dif. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC Intrusions | 2 | 8 | Dif. | 1 | 4 | Dif. | 1 | S/Dif. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC Intrusions-Clues | 2 | Dif. | 3 | 7 | Dif. | 1 | 14 | S/Ded. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC Perseverations | 1 | 3 | S. | 5 | Dif. | 8 | Dif. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC Recog. Correct | 1 | S. | S. | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
TAVEC Recog. False Positive | 5 | Ded. | 5 | 2 | Ded. | 4 | Dif. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
VR I – Total score | 3 | 9 | Dif. | 3 | 9 | 6 | S. | 4 | 17 | 7 | S. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
VR II – Total score | 5 | 7 | S. | 9 | 10 | Ded. | 3 | 14 | 12 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
VR-Copying | S. | 4 | 3 | Dif. | 6 | 4 | 5 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
VR Total Recog. | 7 | 9 | 8 | S. | 10 | 10 | Ded. | 16 | 8 | Ded. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
VR False Positive | 6 | 7 | S. | 1 | 5 | S. | 1 | 8 | Ded. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
VR Visual discrimination | S. | 4 | 2 | Dif. | 7 | 4 | S. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Luria’s HAM Right | 3 | 7 | 5 | S. | 11 | 10 | Dif. | 7 | 16 | 1 | S. | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Luria’s HAM Left | 8 | 7 | S. | 3 | Dif. | 6 | 9 | Dif. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Luria’s – Motor coordination | 12 | Ded. | 5 | 3 | 18 | S/Ded. | 3 | 7 | 14 | S. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Block Design | 6 | 1 | S. | 21 | 9 | 8 | S/Dif. | 34 | 5 | Dif. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Empty cells | 8 | 15 | 19 | 13 | 11 | 18 | 12 | 12 | 18 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Associations | 37 | 30 | 26 | 32 | 34 | 27 | 33 | 33 | 27 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Stable | 53% | 51% | 53% | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Differentiation | 36% | 29% | 33% | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Dedifferentiation | 11% | 20% | 14% | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Importance | NC | <10 | 10 | - | 19 | 20 | - | 29 | >30 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
ME = Middle-age, EE = Early elderly, LE = Late elderly. The explained variance is the total cumulative variance explained by all the predictors in the model. BNT = Boston Naming Test (spontaneous responses). PCV = PC-Vienna System. PASAT = Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test. TMT A = Trial Making Test A. CTT = Color Trails Test. FRT = Facial Recognition Test. JLOT = Judgment of Line Orientation Test. LM = Logical Memory. VR = Visual Reproduction Test. Luria’s HAM = Luria’s Premotor Functions, Hand Alternative Movements. The numbers inside the cells in the “Predictors” area show the importance of each variable in predicting the outcome variable, where the higher the value the higher the importance. The importance is calculated as the relative error in the prediction when a given predictor is excluded from the model. Gray-shaded cells denote that these variables were not important in the model. S.= Stable; Dif. = Differentiation; Ded. = Dedifferentiation; S/Dif. = Stable/Differentiation; S/Ded. = Stable/Dedifferentiation. NC = no contribution. Empty cells = the total number of variables without any contribution. Associations = the total number of variables that are important to predicting verbal fluency. |
Regarding semantic fluency, the most important variables in predicting performance in the middle-age group were CCT, TMT, BNT, PC-Vienna, Logical Memory (Immediate and Delayed), and TAVEC (Learning). In the early elderly group, the most important variables in predicting performance were CCT, TMT, BNT, Logical Memory (Immediate and Delayed), and Stroop. In the late elderly group, the most important variables in predicting performance were CTT, TMT, TAVEC (Learning), Stroop, and Luria’s motor coordination.
Regarding phonemic fluency, the most important variables in predicting performance in the middle-age group were BNT, Stroop, TMT, Digit Span backward, Logical Memory (Immediate), and Block Design. In the early elderly group, the most important variables in predicting performance were BNT, Stroop, TMT, Digit Span forward, and CTT. In the late elderly group, the most important variables in predicting performance were BNT, Stroop, Logical Memory (Immediate), CTT, Luria’s motor coordination, and JLOT.
Regarding action fluency, the most important variables in predicting performance in the middle-age group were TMT, BNT, Logical Memory, Stroop, Block Design, and Digit Span. In the early elderly group, the most important variables in predicting performance were TMT, BNT, Logical Memory, CTT, Visual Reproduction, and Luria’s hand alternative movements. In the late elderly group, the most important variables in predicting performance were Logical Memory, Stroop, CTT, Visual Reproduction, Luria’s motor coordination, TAVEC (Intrusions), and Spatial Span.
Virtually the same results were obtained when including WAIS-III Information and sex as extra predictors in order to investigate their potential confounding effect (data not shown).
Differentiation, dedifferentiation, and stability patterns across age
Three different patterns can be observed in regard to the contribution of cognitive abilities to verbal fluency with increasing age. A differentiation pattern can be observed when cognitive variables stop contributing with increasing age. The dedifferentiation pattern is observed when variables start contributing with increasing age. A stability pattern is seen when the contribution of the variables remains stable across age. In some cases, a combination of these patterns can also be observed in the same variable. We classified as stable/differentiation and stable/dedifferentiation those variables that, despite showing mostly a stability pattern, stop or start contributing with increasing age, respectively. More detail on the procedure to ascertain these patterns is provided in Supplementary Table S2.
Overall, semantic fluency was associated with less cognitive variables with increasing age, indicating a differentiation pattern with aging (Table 3). This is explained because although stability in the associations was observed in 53% of the variables, the percentage of variables showing a differentiation pattern (36%) exceeded the percentage of variables showing a dedifferentiation pattern (11%). In particular, several recall variables of Logical Memory stop contributing to semantic fluency after the early elderly, together with JLOT, PC-Vienna, Digits, Visual Reproduction, and FRT (differentiation). On the other hand, several delayed recall variables of TAVEC as well as Luria’s motor coordination start contributing to semantic fluency in the late elderly (dedifferentiation). Variables with stable contribution are shown in Table 3.
Phonemic fluency showed more stability in the number of cognitive associations with increasing age. The reason for this is that stability in the associations was observed in 51% of the variables, and the percentage of variables showing a differentiation pattern (29%) was rather comparable to the percentage of variables showing a dedifferentiation pattern (20%) with aging, thus cancelling each other. We observed that several recall variables of Logical Memory stop contributing to phonemic fluency after the early elderly, together with TAVEC errors, Visual Reproduction copy and visual discrimination, and Luria’s hand alternative movements (differentiation). In contrast, several delayed recall variables of Visual Reproduction, TAVEC, FRT and CTT start contributing to phonemic fluency in the late elderly (dedifferentiation). Variables with stable contribution are shown in Table 3.
The results in action fluency are a combination of the patterns described above for semantic fluency and phonemic fluency. Stability in the number of cognitive associations prevailed from middle-age to early elderly, while a reduction in the contributing cognitive variables was observed when reaching the late elderly group. This is explained because stability in the associations was observed in a slightly superior proportion of variables as compared with the other two fluency modalities (53%), but the proportion of variables showing a differentiation pattern (33%) exceeded the number of variables showing a dedifferentiation pattern (14%). In particular, several variables of TAVEC stop contributing to action fluency after the early elderly, together with Luria’s motor coordination, JLOT, Block design, Spatial span forward, Digit span backward and FRT (differentiation). The Delayed recall variables of Visual Reproduction and TAVEC start contributing to action fluency in the late elderly, together with JLOT (dedifferentiation).
Discussion
The overall purpose of this study was to investigate how differentiation and dedifferentiation processes in verbal fluency are organized across the lifespan (32 to 84 years). Using cross-sectional data, we investigated the association between performance in three components of verbal fluency (semantic, phonemic, and action) and performance in numerous non-fluency cognitive measures within different age groups from the early middle-age to the late elderly.
Although we found a lower word production with increasing age in the three fluency modalities, age showed a stronger association with semantic fluency than with the other two modalities. The most prominent reduction in performance was observed between the middle-age and the early elderly in the three modalities. At that point in time, a high number of cognitive variables stopped contributing specially to semantic and action fluency. Despite potentially compensatory dedifferentiation patterns in the three modalities, a stronger differentiation process was observed in the three modalities.
The association between age and verbal fluency
Semantic fluency showed a lineal and progressive reduction throughout the whole age range investigated in this study. Other studies including cohorts with a wide range of age have also observed a linear association between age and semantic fluency [39–42]. Some authors have also reported relative stability until the age of 60 [2,16,17,43,44], followed by a decline in performance [1,16,20,44–49]. Therefore, the association between age and semantic fluency is a quite well established finding, although negative reports also exist [30]. Phonemic fluency was rather stable during the middle-age, followed by a drop during the early elderly that seems to get stabilised in the late elderly. Despite these dynamics, our models showed that the linear trend was the best fit (as compared with quadratic and cubic trends). A linear association between age and phonemic fluency has also been observed in previous studies [41,42,50]. Similar to our results, several studies have shown certain stability until the ages of 60-65 years [2,20,43,44], followed by decline [20,39,41,47,49]. However, no association between age and phonemic fluency has also been reported [13,16,26,31,48]. Regarding action fluency, a cubic trend was the best fit in our data. We observed a plateau of high performance during the middle-age, with a drop during the early elderly, and a trend for stability in performance during the late elderly. Previous studies only included elderly individuals and did not found an association between age and action fluency [12,23]. Such finding is in line with the trend for stability in our older age strata.
Fewer studies have simultaneously compared the association between age and the different fluency modalities in the same cohort and statistical model. Indeed, these studies have only compared semantic and phonemic fluency, whereas no data existed on action fluency to the present date. Although we found a significant interaction between age and fluency modality, this result mainly reflects the relationship between action and phonemic fluency. We thus interpret that, in our cohort, semantic and phonemic fluency have a similar association with age. Other groups have also found that semantic and phonemic fluency have a similar association with age [1,39]. However, some studies have shown different results, as for example, an association between semantic fluency and age but not between phonemic fluency and age [13–17,48]. The age range investigated is a major confounder, accounting for part of these contradicting results. Importantly, by covering a wide range of age, from 32 to 84 years, our data help to further understand these discrepancies as well as to delineate the age dynamics in fluency performance.
Differentiation, dedifferentiation, and stability patterns
The contribution of various cognitive functions to verbal fluency performance was different depending on the fluency modality and the age group. Regarding semantic fluency, in the middle-age group, the main contribution was seen for lexical access, processing speed, and verbal memory. In the early elderly group, we observed a greater contribution of executive functions, including working memory, in addition to verbal memory. Lexical access also contributed somehow but the contribution of processing speed was lesser than in the middle-age. Previous studies have found an association of semantic fluency with lexical access [24–26,28], processing speed [32], executive functions [28], and working memory [25]. The novelty of our study is that we reveal age-specific contributions of different cognitive functions to semantic fluency. It is very interesting that the contribution of executive functions was observed in the range of age with greater reduction in word production. This happened in a context of differentiation. This means that when several relevant cognitive functions stop contributing to semantic fluency (differentiation), performance in semantic fluency drops, but new executive components emerge (dedifferentiation), likely being recruited as a compensatory mechanism. This interpretation implies that both differentiation and dedifferentiation patterns can co-occur simultaneously from young ages and not only at the oldest ages, as previously suggested [37,38,51]. New brain networks or new parts of the same networks may be involved in this process, thus extending from the original differentiated function or network. According to the “Compensation-Related Utilization of Neural Circuits Hypothesis” (CRUNCH), the aged brain has to deal with processing inefficiencies and recruits more neuronal resources in order to achieve the same level of performance than a younger brain [52]. This hypothesis is further supported by our results obtained in the late elderly. Executive functions and memory functions remained to contribute to semantic fluency, and premotor functions emerged as a new contributor, also supporting the greater participation of the frontal lobe. The “scaffolding theory of aging and cognition” (STAC) [7] suggests a generalized increased frontal activation with age as a compensatory response. However, we found that the differentiation process is more prominent than the dedifferentiation process. This may be explained by the fact that executive functions can not completely compensate for the onslaught of aging; or this compensation coexists with the overall executive dysfunction observed in normal aging [53]; or both explanations at the same time.
Regarding phonemic fluency, lexical access, working memory, processing speed, and visuoconstructive abilities were the most important contributors in the middle-age. Verbal memory also contributed to a lesser extent. The contribution of visuoconstructive abilities may be explained by the strong executive component of the test used to measure this ability in our cohort. It is possible that shared processes such as planning and processing speed underlie both this test of visuoconstructive abilities [54] and phonetic fluency. The contribution of executive functions on phonemic fluency has been shown in previous studies [28,29,31]. In addition, premotor and visuospatial abilities emerged at the late elderly. Thus, new brain regions seem to be recruited as for semantic fluency (dedifferentiation), but possibly extending more to the posterior cortex in phonetic fluency. The visuoconstructive and visuospatial component of the tasks suggest a greater participation of the right hemisphere with increasing age, in line with the hemispheric asymmetry reduction postulated by the “hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults” (HAROLD) model [55]. The HAROLD effect observed by Cabeza (2002) [55] was interpreted as a compensatory function in which the brain additionally recruits homologous contralateral brain areas [56,57]. Our results suggest that this potential reorganization of the brain is rather effective, minimising the negative onslaught of aging on phonemic fluency, despite how challenging this task can be. This effectiveness contrasts with semantic and action fluency, where we observed a stronger association with age, perhaps due to less effective compensatory mechanisms and a more limited brain reorganisation. Potential explanations for this finding may be that phonemic fluency might be more relevant for the daily life, is more intensively trained during the lifespan, or category strategies are easier [24]. Alternatively, grammatical storages (semantic and actions) may be more vulnerable to aging, while phonemic fluency may allow more flexibility for the activation of different storages through switching strategies [13]. Other researchers have found an association of phonemic fluency with lexical access [29], memory function [31,32], and processing speed [25,26]. Again, the novelty of our study is that we reveal age-specific contributions of different cognitive functions to phonemic fluency.
The functions contributing the most to action fluency in the middle-age were executive functions, processing speed, and verbal memory. In addition to these, visual functions (visual memory and visuospatial functions) started contributing in the older age strata (dedifferentiation). This suggests that new brain networks or parts of the same networks are recruited, including more posterior and right hemispheric regions [55]. The same as for semantic fluency, this finding emerged in the age range with greater reduction in word production, possibly as a compensatory response. We are not aware of studies investigating the association of action fluency with cognitive functions other than episodic memory or picture naming [12,23].
Our interpretations in these last paragraphs regarding cognitive functions underlying different neuropsychological tests are based on the widely used classification of Lezak (2012) [21]. However, neuropsychological tests are known to tap on several cognitive functions, which may reflect that different cognitive functions partially share the same neuronal networks, a finding that would delineate the optimal organization of the human brain (the balance between differentiated and dedifferentiated cognitive components). This organization is adaptive to age-related brain changes and the share of neuronal networks will increase with aging as part of compensatory mechanisms (dedifferentiation).
The present study has some limitations. We analyzed cross-sectional data. Therefore, our age-related differences may partially be explained by cohort effects. We controlled for performance on WAIS-III Information as a means to control for generational effects often overlapped with crystallized intelligence [11,54]. Also, multivariate analysis methods such as random forest have been proven to maximize the covariance between the predictors and the outcome variable, being less vulnerable to confounders such as cohort effects [11]. Nonetheless, we are currently collecting follow-up data so that our present cross-sectional findings can be substantiated in a longitudinal design. In addition, future studies should further disentangle the mechanisms behind co-occurring differentiation and dedifferentiation processes. For example, how executive functioning substitutes the contribution of other cognitive functions, in a context of overall executive dysfunction as individuals age, needs to be further investigated. Fluency performance varies according to the type of stimulus (either letter or a category) [58]. Therefore, it is warranted to replicate our current findings using other stimulus for semantic (e.g. vegetables) and phonemic fluency (e.g. C-F-L). In this study we focused on high order cognitive functions. However, previous studies have shown that dedifferentiation findings extend to peripheral sensorimotor abilities such as visual and auditory acuity [59], which deserves further attention in the future. Also, we focused on the contribution of non-language functions (other than lexical access: BNT) towards the prediction of verbal fluency. Therefore, investigating the contribution of non-fluency language components towards the prediction of verbal fluency is warranted in future studies. The association between age and cognition is largely determined by biological changes taking place in the brain during aging. Therefore, extending our analyses to neuroimaging measures in the future is warranted and might help to better understand the neural correlates of our current findings.
Conclusions
Verbal fluency declines with increasing age. Semantic fluency seems to be more vulnerable to aging than phonemic and action fluency. However, these dynamics are masked by the influence of other cognitive functions, which may themselves be declining with age as well. Lexical access, processing speed, and executive functions are among the most contributing functions. The most striking contribution of new cognitive functions takes place during the transition from the middle-age to the early elderly. Differentiation processes (functions stop contributing with increasing age) coexist with dedifferentiation processes (new functions start contributing with increasing age). Compensatory mechanisms are postulated to underlie these patterns. All in all, we present important data towards advancing to a better understanding of cognitive aging and compensatory processes. These findings may be relevant for personalizing age-specific cognitive interventions by guiding the development of materials for cognitive stimulation and/or rehabilitation in the close future. This knowledge may also be relevant for the clinical practice, improving interpretation of cognitive performance, and eventually improving diagnosis of cognitive disorders. Furthermore, our research could easily be extended to the study of other cognitive functions.
Methods
Participants
A total of 446 participants were selected from the GENIC-database (Group of Neuropsychological Studies of the Canary Islands) [11], with ages between 32 and 84 years, and a balanced distribution of sex across age. All participants were evaluated with a comprehensive neuropsychological protocol, which assesses language, processing speed, attention, executive functions, verbal and visual episodic memory, procedural memory, and visuoconstructive, visuoperceptive and visuospatial functions (see Supplementary Table S1 and [60,61] for detailed information about the protocol). Inclusion criteria were: (1) normal cognitive performance in comprehensive neuropsychological assessment using pertinent clinical normative data (i.e. individuals with mild cognitive impairment or dementia were excluded); (2) preserved global cognitive and functional status operationalized as a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score ≥24, a Blessed Dementia Scale (BDRS) score <4 and/or a Functional Activity Questionnaire (FAQ) score <6); (3) no neurologic, psychiatric or systemic diseases; and (4) no history of substance abuse. An exception was done for BDRS. Although the BDRS scale cut-off for abnormality is frequently established at ≥4 points [62,63], the ‘changes in personality, interests and drive’ subscale may influence the BDRS total score and does not necessary reflect functional impairment. With the objective of excluding only individuals with functional impairment, we included those participants with total BDRS scores ≥4 (n=24) if: a) 70% or higher percentage of the BDRS total score resulted from the ‘changes in personality, interests and drive’ subscale; and b) if a score ≤1.5 was obtained in the other two subscales (‘changes in performance of everyday activities’ and ‘changes in habits’). The same procedure has been used in previous studies [11,64]. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of La Laguna (Spain) and all participants gave their written informed consent.
Neuropsychological assessment
Among all the tests included in our neuropsychological protocol, three tests of verbal fluency are of special relevance for the current study:
Phonemic verbal fluency
The Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT [65];) was administrated. Participants had to recall words that begin with the letters F, A, and S, taking one minute on each of the letters. Proper nouns, numbers, and derived words were considered intrusion errors. A total score (F+A+S) was calculated as the number of correct words produced, excluding intrusions and perseverations (repetitions of correct words).
Semantic verbal fluency
Instructions were given following the administration procedures described in the Multilingual Aphasia Examination [65]. Participants had to recall names of animals during one minute. The total number of words, perseverations, and intrusions were registered.
Action verbal fluency
Participants had to recall verbs in infinitive form (e.g. “to reflect”). Verbs included as part of a sentence (e.g. “to dance the tango”) and repetitions of the same verb were considered errors [12]. The total number of correct verbs, intrusions, and perseverations were counted.
Other neuropsychological tests selected for this study are explained in Supplementary Table S1, including Information Subtest (from the WAIS-III), Boston Naming Test (BNT), PC-Vienna System, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT), Stroop Test, Trail Making Test (TMT), Colour Trial Test (CTT), Facial Recognition Test (FRT), Judgment of Line Orientation Test (JLOT), Digit Span (from the WMS-III), Visuospatial Span (from the WMS-III), Logical Memory (LM, from the WMS-III), “Test de Aprendizaje Verbal España-Complutense” (TAVEC, the Spanish adaptation and validation of the California Verbal Learning Test), Visual Reproduction (VR, from the WMS-III), Luria’s Premotor Functions (“hand alternative movements” and “motor coordination”), and Block Design (from the WAIS-III).
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using the R programming environment [66]. The association between age (between-subject factor, 3 or 4 age groups) and verbal fluency (within-subject factor, 3 fluency modalities) was tested using mixed ANCOVA, including WAIS-III Information as a covariable in order to control for between-subjects variability in the level of crystallized intelligence [11]. With the aim of investigating potential non-linear associations between age and performance in verbal fluency, we tested for quadratic and cubic associations in addition to linear associations in the mixed ANCOVA. To do this, we used the technique “trend analysis”, which is a way of decomposing the variance explained by the factor that accompanies an ANOVA using specially chosen linear weights called “orthogonal polynomials”. The polynomial contrast will test for trends in the data depending on the number of levels of the numeric factor. Since we have more than two levels in our independent variable, the polynomial contrast will examine other trends that can exist in the data such as quadratic and cubic trends. Random forest regression analyses were used to investigate the multivariate association between the measures of verbal fluency and a total of 45 cognitive variables. In random forest models, the contribution of the predictors in the models is reported as Imp (from Importance), which reflects the relative error in the prediction when a predictor is excluded from the model. Imp values higher than zero denote that a given variable contributes to the prediction of the outcome. The larger the Imp value, the greater the contribution. Imp values do not have an upper limit and they can rather be interpreted by considering the obtained values in relation to the variable yielding the highest Imp value in the model. Two per cent of the values were missing across the 48 cognitive variables and were thus imputed. Only the random forest analyses were performed on this imputed dataset. For the demographic variables, ANOVA was used for both continuous and dichotomous (dummy) variables. Simple regression analysis was performed to investigate the association between pairs of continuous variables. Significant differences were considered when p<0.05.
Supplementary Materials
Supplementary Tables
Acknowledgements
Data used in the preparation of this article is part of the GENIC-database (Group of Neuropsychological Studies of the Canary Islands, University of La Laguna, Spain. Principal investigator: Professor José Barroso. Contact: Dr. Daniel Ferreira, daniel.ferreira.padilla@ki.se). The following collaborators contributed to the GENIC-database but did not participate in analysis or writing of this report (in alphabetic order by family name): Nira Cedrés, Rut Correia, Patricia Díaz, Nerea Figueroa, Eloy García, Teodoro González, Zaira González, Cathaysa Hernández, Edith Hernández, Nira Jiménez, Judith López, Alejandra Machado, Yaiza Molina, María Sabucedo, Elena Sirumal, Marta Suárez, Manuel Urbano, and Pedro Velasco.
Conflicts of Interest
All the authors declared no conflicts of interest relevant to the current study.
Funding
This study was funded by the Agencia Canaria de Investigación, Innovación y Sociedad de la Información de la Consejería de Economía, Industria, Comercio y Conocimiento y por el Fondo Social Europeo (FSE) Programa Operativo Integrado de Canarias 2014-2020, Eje 3 Tema Prioritario 74 (85%); Fundación Cajacanarias; the Swedish Foundation for Strategic Research (SSF), the Strategic Research Programme in Neuroscience at Karolinska Institutet (StratNeuro), the Swedish Research Council (VR), the Åke Wiberg foundation, Hjärnfonden, Alzheimerfonden, Demensfonden, Stiftelsen Olle Engkvist Byggmästare, and Birgitta och Sten Westerberg. The funders of the study had no role in the study design nor the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, writing of the report, or decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
References
- 1. Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Glymour MM, Elbaz A, Berr C, Ebmeier KP, Ferrie JE, Dugravot A. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2012; 344:d7622–7622. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d7622 [PubMed]
- 2. Ferreira D, Correia R, Nieto A, Machado A, Molina Y, Barroso J. Cognitive decline before the age of 50 can be detected with sensitive cognitive measures. Psicothema. 2015; 27:216–22. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2014.192 [PubMed]
- 3. Harada CN, Natelson Love MC, Triebel KL. Normal cognitive aging. Clin Geriatr Med. 2013; 29:737–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002 [PubMed]
- 4. Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging. 2009; 30:507–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023 [PubMed]
- 5. Salthouse TA, Fristoe NM, Lineweaver TT, Coon VE. Aging of attention: does the ability to divide decline? Mem Cognit. 1995; 23:59–71. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210557 [PubMed]
- 6. Salthouse TA. Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010; 16:754–60. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710000706 [PubMed]
- 7. Park DC, Reuter-Lorenz P. The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009; 60:173–96. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656 [PubMed]
- 8. La Tourette TR, Meeks S. Perceptions of patronizing speech by older women in nursing homes and in the community: impact of cognitive ability and place of residence. J Lang Soc Psychol. 2000; 19:463–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X00019004004
- 9. Shafto MA, Tyler LK. Language in the aging brain: the network dynamics of cognitive decline and preservation. Science. 2014; 346:583–87. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1254404 [PubMed]
- 10. Ansado J, Marsolais Y, Methqal I, Alary F, Joanette Y. The adaptive aging brain: evidence from the preservation of communication abilities with age. Eur J Neurosci. 2013; 37:1887–95. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.12252 [PubMed]
- 11. Machado A, Barroso J, Molina Y, Nieto A, Díaz-Flores L, Westman E, Ferreira D. Proposal for a hierarchical, multidimensional, and multivariate approach to investigate cognitive aging. Neurobiol Aging. 2018; 71:179–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.07.017 [PubMed]
- 12. Piatt AL, Fields JA, Paolo AM, Tröster AI. Action (verb naming) fluency as an executive function measure: convergent and divergent evidence of validity. Neuropsychologia. 1999; 37:1499–503. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00066-4 [PubMed]
- 13. Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G. Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology. 1997; 11:138–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.138 [PubMed]
- 14. Parkin AJ, Java RI. Deterioration of frontal lobe function in normal aging: influences of fluid intelligence versus perceptual speed. Neuropsychology. 1999; 13:539–45. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.13.4.539 [PubMed]
- 15. Kemper S, Sumner A. The structure of verbal abilities in young and older adults. Psychol Aging. 2001; 16:312–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.312 [PubMed]
- 16. Foldi NS, Helm-Estabrooks N, Redfield J, Nickel DG. Perseveration in normal aging: A comparison of perseveration rates on design fluency and verbal generative tasks. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2003; 10:268–80. https://doi.org/10.1076/anec.10.4.268.28970
- 17. Elgamal SA, Roy EA, Sharratt MT. Age and verbal fluency: the mediating effect of speed of processing. Can Geriatr J. 2011; 14:66–72. https://doi.org/10.5770/cgj.v14i3.17 [PubMed]
- 18. Troyer AK. Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000; 22:370–78. https://doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200006)22:3;1-V;FT370 [PubMed]
- 19. Loonstra AS, Tarlow AR, Sellers AH. COWAT metanorms across age, education, and gender. Appl Neuropsychol. 2001; 8:161–66. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324826AN0803_5 [PubMed]
- 20. Rodriguez-Aranda C, Martinussen M. Age-related differences in performance of phonemic verbal fluency measured by Controlled Oral Word Association Task (COWAT): a meta-analytic study. Dev Neuropsychol. 2006; 30:697–717. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn3002_3 [PubMed]
- 21. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Bigler ED, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment, 5th ed. 5th ed. Neuropsychological assessment. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press; 2012.
- 22. Bryan J, Luszcz MA. Measurement of executive function: considerations for detecting adult age differences. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000; 22:40–55. https://doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200002)22:1;1-8;FT040 [PubMed]
- 23. Piatt AL, Fields JA, Paolo AM, Tröster AI. Action verbal fluency normative data for the elderly. Brain Lang. 2004; 89:580–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.02.003 [PubMed]
- 24. Kavé G, Mashal N. Age-related differences in word-retrieval but not in meaning generation. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2012; 19:515–29. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2011.638975 [PubMed]
- 25. Kraan C, Stolwyk RJ, Testa R. The abilities associated with verbal fluency performance in a young, healthy population are multifactorial and differ across fluency variants. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2013; 20:159–68. https://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2012.670157 [PubMed]
- 26. Stolwyk R, Bannirchelvam B, Kraan C, Simpson K. The cognitive abilities associated with verbal fluency task performance differ across fluency variants and age groups in healthy young and old adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2015; 37:70–83. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2014.988125 [PubMed]
- 27. Lezak M, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2004.
- 28. Shao Z, Janse E, Visser K, Meyer AS. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Front Psychol. 2014; 5:772. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772 [PubMed]
- 29. Ruff RM, Light RH, Parker SB, Levin HS. The psychological construct of word fluency. Brain Lang. 1997; 57:394–405. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1997.1755 [PubMed]
- 30. Rodríguez-Aranda C, Sundet K. The frontal hypothesis of cognitive aging: factor structure and age effects on four frontal tests among healthy individuals. J Genet Psychol. 2006; 167:269–87. https://doi.org/10.3200/GNTP.167.3.269-287 [PubMed]
- 31. Bolla KI, Lindgren KN, Bonaccorsy C, Bleecker ML. Predictors of verbal fluency (FAS) in the healthy elderly. J Clin Psychol. 1990; 46:623–28. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(199009)46:5<623::AID-JCLP2270460513>3.0.CO;2-C [PubMed]
- 32. Ardila A, Galeano LM, Rosselli M. Toward a model of neuropsychological activity. Neuropsychol Rev. 1998; 8:171–90. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021618218943 [PubMed]
- 33. Garrett HE. A developmental theory of intelligence. Am Psychol. 1946; 1:372–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0056380 [PubMed]
- 34. Balinsky B. An analysis of the mental factors of various age groups from nine to sixty. Genet Psychol Monogr. 1941; 23:191–234.
- 35. Baltes PB, Cornelius SW, Spiro A, Nesselroade JR, Willis SL. Integration versus differentiation of fluid/crytallized intelligence in old age. Dev Psychol. 1980; 16:625–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.16.6.625
- 36. Baltes PB, Lindenberger U. Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: a new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychol Aging. 1997; 12:12–21. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.12.1.12 [PubMed]
- 37. Hülür G, Ram N, Willis SL, Schaie KW, Gerstorf D. Cognitive dedifferentiation with increasing age and proximity of death: Within-person evidence from the Seattle Longitudinal Study. Psychol Aging. 2015; 30:311–23. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039260 [PubMed]
- 38. La Fleur CG, Meyer MJ, Dodson C. Exploring dedifferentiation across the adult lifespan. Psychol Aging. 2018; 33:855–70. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000274 [PubMed]
- 39. Bolla KI, Gray S, Resnick SM, Galante R, Kawas C. Category and Letter Fluency in Highly Educated Older Adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 1998; 12:330–38. https://doi.org/10.1076/clin.12.3.330.1986
- 40. Brickman AM, Paul RH, Cohen RA, Williams LM, MacGregor KL, Jefferson AL, Tate DF, Gunstad J, Gordon E. Category and letter verbal fluency across the adult lifespan: relationship to EEG theta power. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2005; 20:561–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2004.12.006 [PubMed]
- 41. Kavé G, Knafo-Noam A. Lifespan development of phonemic and semantic fluency: universal increase, differential decrease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2015; 37:751–63. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2015.1065958 [PubMed]
- 42. Olabarrieta-Landa L, Rivera D, Galarza-Del-Angel J, Garza MT, Saracho CP, Rodríguez W, Chávez-Oliveros M, Rábago B, Leibach G, Schebela S, Martínez C, Luna M, Longoni M, et al. Verbal fluency tests: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation. 2015; 37:515–61. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-151279 [PubMed]
- 43. Daigneault S, Braun CM, Whitaker HA. Early effects of normal aging on perseverative and non‐perseverative prefrontal measures. Dev Neuropsychol. 1992; 8:99–114. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649209540518
- 44. Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999; 14:167–77. [PubMed]
- 45. Kempler D, Teng EL, Dick M, Taussig IM, Davis DS. The effects of age, education, and ethnicity on verbal fluency. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998; 4:531–38. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617798466013 [PubMed]
- 46. Ravdin LD, Katzen HL, Agrawal P, Relkin NR. Letter and semantic fluency in older adults: effects of mild depressive symptoms and age-stratified normative data. Clin Neuropsychol. 2003; 17:195–202. https://doi.org/10.1076/clin.17.2.195.16500 [PubMed]
- 47. Auriacombe S, Fabrigoule C, Lafont S, Jacqmin-Gadda H, Dartigues JF. Letter and Category Fluency in Normal Elderly Participants: a Population-Based Study. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2001; 8:98–108. https://doi.org/10.1076/anec.8.2.98.841
- 48. Crossley M, D’Arcy C, Rawson NS. Letter and category fluency in community-dwelling Canadian seniors: a comparison of normal participants to those with dementia of the Alzheimer or vascular type. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1997; 19:52–62. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639708403836 [PubMed]
- 49. Bryan J, Luszcz MA, Crawford JR. Verbal knowledge and speed of information processing as mediators of age differences in verbal fluency performance among older adults. Psychol Aging. 1997; 12:473–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.12.3.473 [PubMed]
- 50. Bäckman L, Nilsson LG. Semantic Memory Functioning Across the Adult Life Span. Eur Psychol. 1996; 1:27–33. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.1.1.27
- 51. Wilson RS, Segawa E, Hizel LP, Boyle PA, Bennett DA. Terminal dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities. Neurology. 2012; 78:1116–22. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e31824f7ff2 [PubMed]
- 52. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Cappell KA. Neurocognitive Aging and the Compensation Hypothesis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2008; 17:177–82. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00570.x
- 53. Dempster FN. The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Dev Rev. 1992; 12:45–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(92)90003-K
- 54. Ferreira D, Bartrés-Faz D, Nygren L, Rundkvist LJ, Molina Y, Machado A, Junqué C, Barroso J, Westman E. Different reserve proxies confer overlapping and unique endurance to cortical thinning in healthy middle-aged adults. Behav Brain Res. 2016; 311:375–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.05.061 [PubMed]
- 55. Cabeza R. Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol Aging. 2002; 17:85–100. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.85 [PubMed]
- 56. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Lustig C. Brain aging: reorganizing discoveries about the aging mind. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005; 15:245–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.016 [PubMed]
- 57. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. Human neuroscience and the aging mind: a new look at old problems. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010; 65:405–15. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbq035 [PubMed]
- 58. Diaz M, Sailor K, Cheung D, Kuslansky G. Category size effects in semantic and letter fluency in Alzheimer’s patients. Brain Lang. 2004; 89:108–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00307-9 [PubMed]
- 59. Lindenberger U, Ghisletta P. Cognitive and sensory declines in old age: gauging the evidence for a common cause. Psychol Aging. 2009; 24:1–16. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014986 [PubMed]
- 60. Ferreira D, Molina Y, Machado A, Westman E, Wahlund LO, Nieto A, Correia R, Junqué C, Díaz-Flores L, Barroso J. Cognitive decline is mediated by gray matter changes during middle age. Neurobiol Aging. 2014; 35:1086–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.10.095 [PubMed]
- 61. Correia R, Nieto A, Ferreira D, Sabucedo M, Barroso J. Fund of Information is More Strongly Associated with Neuropsychological Functioning Than Education in Older Spanish Adults. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2015; 30:310–21. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acv011 [PubMed]
- 62. Blessed G. Tomlinson Be, Roth M. The Association Between Quantitative Measures of Dementia and of Senile Change in the Cerebral Grey Matter of Elderly Subjects. Br J Psychiatry. 1968; 114:797–811. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.114.512.797 [PubMed]
- 63. Erkinjuntti T, Hokkanen L, Sulkava R, Palo J. The Blessed Dementia Scale as a screening test for dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1988; 3:267–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.930030406
- 64. Cedres N, Machado A, Molina Y, Diaz-Galvan P, Hernández-Cabrera JA, Barroso J, Westman E, Ferreira D. Subjective Cognitive Decline Below and Above the Age of 60: A Multivariate Study on Neuroimaging, Cognitive, Clinical, and Demographic Measures. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019; 68:295–309. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-180720 [PubMed]
- 65. Benton A, Hamsher K, Sivan A. Multilingual aphasia examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates. 2nd Ed. Associates A, editor. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa; 1989.
- 66. Core TR. A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria; 2016.